Journal of Behavior Therapy And Mental Health

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Mental Health

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Mental Health

Current Issue Volume No: 1 Issue No: 3

Research Article Open Access Available online freely Peer Reviewed Citation

Childhood Somatic Complaints: Relationships with Child Emotional Functioning and Parental Factors

1Research Institute Child Development and Education, University of Amsterdam

Abstract

Many schoolchildren experience somatic complaints such as headaches, abdominal pain and fatigue. The aim of the current research is to test the full model of previously found associations between negative affect and somatic complaints in parents and children. Participants were 199 children (aged 8-13, 47% boys) and their parents (aged 31-61, mostly mothers (87%). Self-reports of children and parents on worry, anxiety, depression and somatic complaints were used and parents’ reactions to children’s emotions wereassessed. The results of the study show that childhood negative affect and parental somatic complaints are positively associated with childhood somatic complaints. In turn, childhood negative affect is related to children’s worrying and to parents’ responses to children’s emotions. The more anxious or depressed children felt, the more they worried. Maladaptive parental responses (such as reprimands and discomfort) to child emotions were positively related to depression. It was also found that parents who experienced more negative affect, reported more somatic complaints and tended to report more maladaptive responses towards their children’s emotions.

Author Contributions
Received 15 Jun 2016; Accepted 29 Sep 2016; Published 04 Oct 2016;

Academic Editor: Pierre Chuard, Department of Biology, Concordia University, 7141 Sherbrooke Street West, Montreal, QC, H4B 1R6, Canada

Checked for plagiarism: Yes

Review by: Single-blind

Copyright ©  2016 Francine C. Jellesma, et al.

License
Creative Commons License     This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Competing interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Citation:

Francine C. Jellesma (2016) Childhood Somatic Complaints: Relationships with Child Emotional Functioning and Parental Factors. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Mental Health - 1(3):14-26. https://doi.org/10.14302/issn.2474-9273.jbtm-16-1173

Download as RIS, BibTeX, Text (Include abstract )

DOI 10.14302/issn.2474-9273.jbtm-16-1173

Introduction

Many schoolchildren experience somatic complaints such as headaches, abdominal pain and fatigue: prevalence rates range between 10 and 30% for recurrent or chronic complaints 1, 2, 3, 4, 5. These complaints frequently are associated with psychological problems 6, 7, 8. A significant amount of research during the past few decades has been devoted to determining relationships between various psychological factors and childhood somatic complaints 9. Some researchers have focused on the positive relationship between negative affect and somatic complaints 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16. Other researchers have taken an interest in parental factors that could cause somatic complaints in children 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25. These two lines of research have resulted in research outcomes that are theoretically compatible. Still, they have not yet been integrated.

The Relationship Between Negative Affect and Somatic Complaints

Negative emotions or stress have a physiological component: the body responds in order to enable a person to fight or flight from situations that cause negative emotions or stress. According to the biobehavioral model of pediatric pain and the perseverative cognition hypothesis, negative affect in the form of depression, anxiety or worrying, is an intensified and prolonged psychological state 12, 14. The belonging physiological responses that are present because of this negative affect result in the experience of somatic complaints 12, 14. Previous research clearly shows that children with symptoms of depression or anxiety indeed more often report somatic complaints than their peers without these symptoms 7, 10, 11, 12. Similarly, it has been shown that children who frequently worry experience more complaints than their peers 13. Whereas anxiety, depression, and worrying are strongly related 15, 16, research suggests that at least anxiety and depression have a unique relationship with somatic complaints in children and adults 8, 17, 18. The unique role of worrying is less clear; worrying as a minimum is a strong predictor of negative affect 16. All three variables were therefore included in the current study.

Parental Influences on Childhood Somatic Complaints

With respect to the second line of research, several studies have demonstrated a positive relationship between the occurrence of parental somatic complaints (particularly maternal) and child somatic complaints 23, 24. This relationship is probably partly explained genetically, in the sense that children inherit a genetic disposition to more easily develop somatic complaints 19. Further, it has been suggested that parents may have a more indirect influence on children’s somatic complaints by their parenting behavior. For example, early low perceived parental control and insecure attachment have been found to predict later experiences of somatic complaints in children 25.

It is essential to understand that the relationship between negative affect and somatic complaints described above, is not just applicable to children, it is also applicable to adults. Thus, those parents who experience somatic complaints, also often experience negative affect. Parental negative affect is seen as a risk factor for children’s functioning. After all, parents with negative affect can respond in maladaptive ways to their children. More precisely, previous research has shown that children whose mothers respond to emotions in an invalidating (e.g., restrictive or punishing) rather than validating (e.g., encouraging expression) way, have more problems with emotion regulation and experience more feelings of anxiety or depression than their peers 26, 27, 28. From these previous studies, it can be concluded that children feel better when parents acknowledge their emotions and respond empathically or supportive than when parents become distressed or try to minimize emotion expression by the child 29. Theoretically, this would also cause somatic complaints in children, but this has not yet been empirically verified.

Summarizing the Results from Previous Research

In conclusion, the current literature has demonstrated that negative affect is associated with somatic complaints 7, 10, 11, 12, 13. Further, somatic complaints in parents are associated with somatic complaints in children 23, 24. In addition, parents with somatic complaints also often suffer from negative affect 17. In turn, negative affect in parents is associated with invalidating responses to children’s emotions. These invalidating responses to emotions are associated with negative affect in children 26, 27, 28, 29. What has not yet been studied, however, is the full model that includes both the direct effect of parental somatic complaints on children’s somatic complaints as well as the indirect effects of negative affect in parents, as depicted in Figure 1: because parental negative affect is associated with maladaptive responses to children’s emotions, this results in negative affect in children, that again is associated with children’s somatic complaints. In the current study, a first step was made to combine the two lines of research (i.e., childhood negative affect and parental factors in association with childhood somatic complaints).

Figure 1.Theoretical explanation of the direct association between somatic complaints in parents and children (black) and the indirect pathway (grey)
 Theoretical explanation of the direct association between somatic complaints in parents and children (black) and the indirect pathway (grey)

The Current Study

In the current study the aim was to address this full model of direct and indirect associations between parents’ negative affect, emotional responses and somatic complaints and children’s negative affect and somatic complaints. Simultaneously studying different types of potential influences on children’s somatic complaints has the advantage of separating independent effects of each factor that could otherwise be overestimated Based on the above described previous findings,the following associations were expected:

a positive relationship between depression, anxiety, worrying and somatic complaints for children as well as parents

a direct, positive relationship between parental somatic complaints and child somatic complaints

positive relationships between parental negative affect and maladaptive reactions to child emotions

positive relationships between maladaptive maternal reactions to child emotions and negative affect in children

Method

Participants

Participants were 199 children (aged 8-13, mean age=10; 47% boys) and their parents (aged 31-61, mean age = 42). Almost all parental questionnaires were completed by mothers (87%). The vast majority of the families were from original Dutch descent (90%; other common ethnic backgrounds were Surinam and Indonesian). Parental levels of education were representative of the Dutch population.

Procedure

Data collection was part of a larger research project. Sixteen 16 random schools in North Holland were asked to participate. Depending on school size, a minimum of one and a maximum of three classrooms were asked to participate. These were 33 classrooms grade 4 to 6 (Dutch school system group 6-8), with the inclusion of one combination classroom grades 3 /4. Parents received an information letter and were asked for informed consent. Parental consent was received for 73% of the children (n = 600). They were tested in their own classroom at school (this larger data set will be used for a different study). Parents were asked to send their own questionnaires in a prepaid envelop to the University. Of the parent quationnaire, 199 were received that could be used for data analysis; an additional 10% was unusable because parents had left the identification part of the questionnaire empty. No differences were found in child reports of depression, anxiety, worry or somatic complaints between the group with and the group without parental participation.

Measures

Children

The Children’s Depression Inventory short form is a 10-item self-reported depressive symptoms 30. Prior research has supported the validity and reliability of the Dutch version for the current age group 31. For each item, participants are asked to select one of the three statements that best describes the way that the participant has been feeling during the past 2 weeks. For each item, scores are 0 = absence of symptom, 1 = mild symptom, and 2=definite symptom. The internal consistency was .66.

The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children is a 39-item self-report instrument that screens for various anxiety problems experienced by children and adolescents 8–19 years of age 32. Previous studies have found support for the validity and reliability of the Dutch scales in middle childhood 33, 34. Four basic scales are included: Social Anxiety, Physical Symptoms, Harm Avoidance, and Separation/Panic. Items are on a 3-point scale 0(never true about me) to 3 (often true about me). All subscales had sufficient internal consistencies of .65-.85. For the analyses, the Physical Symptoms subscale was excluded because of overlap in content with childhood somatic complaints (r = .67, p < .01).

The Non-Productive Thought Questionnaire-Kids consists of 10 statements, with a 3-point scale from 0 (never true to) 2 = often true 27. Items measure worry/rumination, regardless of content (e.g., If I have a problem, I cannot stop thinking about it). The internal consistency of the questionnaire was .79.

The Somatic Complaint List consists of 11 common somatic complaints that are rated on a 5-point scale (from 1=(almost) never to 5=quite often) 35. The internal consistency was .76.

Parents

The Parents’ Reaction to Children’s Positive Emotions Scale consists of a series of 12 vignettes in which children are likely to experience positive emotions such as joy, pride, excitement, and curiosity 26, 27, 28. The PRCPS includes different contexts (with peers or acquaintances, at a birthday party, in a car, etc.), but the parent is always present. For each situation, parents indicate on a 7-point scale (from 1=very unlikely to 7=very likely) how likely they would be to react as described in each of the six alternative responses. The PRCPS yields four subscales: Socialization, reflecting the degree to which parents explain to their child the reasons why their expressive behavior may be inappropriate given social norms or etiquette; Encouragement, indicating the degree to which parents encourage their child to express positive emotions or validate their child’s positive emotional states; Reprimand, reflecting the degree to which parents react by reprimanding their child for expressing positive emotions; and Discomfort, indicating the degree to which parents feel discomfort, embarrassed, or irritated when their child expresses positive emotions.

The Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale36 consists of12 typical situations in which young children are described as experiencing distress and negative affect (e.g., being teased by peers, being scared of injections, being nervous about possibly embarrassing him/herself in public). Similar to the PRCPS, for each situation, the parent is asked to indicate how likely (on a 7-point scale) he or she would be to react in each of six different ways. The CCNES yields six subscales: Punitive Reactions; reflecting the degree to which parents respond with punitive, controlling responses that decrease their exposure or need to deal with the negative emotions of their children; Minimization Reactions, reflecting the degree to which parents minimize the seriousness of the situation or devalue the child’s problem or emotional response; Distress Reactions, reflecting the degree to which parents experience negative emotional arousal when children express negative; Expressive Encouragement, reflecting the degree to which parents encourage children to express negative affect or the degree to which they validate child's negative emotional states, Emotion-Focused Reactions reflecting the degree to which parents respond with strategies that are designed to help the child feel better; and Problem-Focused Reactions, reflecting the degree to which parents help the child solve the problem that caused the child's distress.

The PRCPS and the CCNES questionnaires were presented to parents as one integrated questionnaire. All subscales had sufficient internal consistencies of .60-.85, although 4 items had to be removed in order to achieve this, which is consistent with previous research 27.

The Beck Depression Inventory is a 21-item scale designed to measure the presence of emotional, behavioural and somatic symptoms of depression over the previous week 37. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 to 3. The internal consistency was .80.

The State Trait Anxiety Inventory- Trait scale consists of 20 items that predominantly measure affective and cognitive components of anxiety that are more stable rather than situational 38 . Participants rate each item on a scale of 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). The internal consistency was 0.92.

The Penn State Worry Questionnaire is a 16-item questionnaire that assesses the generality, excessiveness, and uncontrollability of worry, regardless of content 39. Individuals rate each item on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (very true). The internal consistency was 0.95.

Parents also completed the Somatic Complaint List. The internal consistency was .84.

Statistical Analyses

The data were analyzed with structural equation modeling. In our preliminary analyses, a measurement model was fitted. For the parental reactions, a principal component analyses was first performed in order to determine underlying dimensions. For the other variables, the (sub)scales were used as indicators.

Measurement error variances of the constructs that did not have multiple indicators were fixed based on the reliability estimate. A comparative fit index (CFI) > .90 and a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation of < .80 were considered as indicators of adequate model fit 40, 41. Then, the relationships were further explored, guided by the hypotheses, but build up in steps in order to carefully determine direct and mediating effects. For each subsequent model, the model fit was evaluated. For the evaluation of each pathway between the constructs, the z-test was used (with a critical z value of |1.96|).

Results

Preliminary Analyses

InTable 1, the descriptives are presented as well as the simple correlation between the parent and child variables. A principal component analyses with varimax rotation was carried out on the subscales of the Parental Reactions to Children’s Positive Emotions subscales and the Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions subscales. The scree plot indicated a two factor structure. Based on this analysis, two latent constructs were defined, with the indicators in parenthesis: Invalidating responses (distress reactions, punitive reactions, minimization reactions, discomfort, and reprimand) and Validating responses (emotion-focused reactions, problem focused reactions, expressive encouragement, and encouragement). Socializing reactions loaded on both factors and were not used in further analyses. For childhood anxiety, the anxiety subscales harm avoidance, separation anxiety, and social anxiety were used. The other variables were represented by a single indicator. The fixed the error variance was fixed on (1-relaibility)*variance of the scale. In the first measurement model, all latent variables were allowed to covary. This model did not reach acceptable model fit, χ2(114) = 322.42, CFI = .866, RMSEA = .096. Based on the similarity in content, covariation was then allowed between distress reactions and discomfort; expressive encouragement and encouragement; and punitive reactions and minimalizing reactions. This second model resulted in a good model fit, χ2(111) = 249.02, CFI = .911, RMSEA = .079.

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of All Variabels and Correlations Between the Parent and Child Variables
      Child Variables          
      Somatic Complaints Depression Social Anxiety Harm avoidance Seperation/panic Worry
Parent Variables     M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
M (SD) 1.90 (0.51) 0.26 (0.23) 2.00 (0.64) 2.89 (0.45) 1.82 (0.51) 0.91 (0.46)
Somatic Complaints 1.72 -0.54 .23** 0.07 0.09 0.03 .15* 0.01
Depression 0.22 -0.22 0.12 0.06 0.09 0 0.1 0.05
Anxiety 1.59 -0.43 .15* .14* 0.01 -0.03 0.05 -0.04
Worry 2.2 -0.68 .17* .19** 0.03 0.02 -0.01 0.02
Discomfort 2.21 -0.82 0.02 .14* 0.07 0.01 0.07 0.14
Reprimand 3.38 -0.93 -0.04 0.09 0.07 -0.03 0.03 0.08
Encouragement 4.27 -0.86 0 -.18** -0.06 0.11 -0.05 0.01
Socialization 4.96 -0.81 -0.07 0.09 0.08 0.03 0.11 0.08
Distress 2.03 -0.65 0.09 .19** 0.06 -0.06 0.01 0.13
Punitive 2.08 -0.62 0.06 .16* 0.05 0.01 0.05 0.1
Minimization 2.72 -0.87 0.05 0.05 0.08 0.07 0.09 0.11
Expressive Encouragement 4.73 -1 0.03 -0.08 0.02 .15* 0.08 -0.01
Emotion Focused 5.19 -0.88 -0.05 -0.08 0.05 .16* 0.01 0
Problem Focused 5.66 -0.79 -0.01 -0.09 0.05 0.11 0.01 -0.07

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Model Testing

Our analyses showed that worrying had no direct effect on childhood somatic complaints (z=0.26), whereas depression (z= 2.10) and anxiety (z=3.29) both predicted more somatic complaints. Notice that, as expected, worry was positively associated with anxiety and depression.

With respect to the parent variables, the only significant predictor of childhood somatic complaints was parental somatic complaints (z=2.07). It was alsotried to predict parental somatic complaints out of parental anxiety, depression, and worrying and a significant path for anxiety was found (z=2.09).

With respect to the question whether childhood depression and anxiety could be predicted by parental depression, anxiety, and/or parental reactions to child emotions; it was found that for child depression, positive (z=-2.03) and negative reactions (z=2.14) were significant predictors. Finally, it was examined whether parental reactions could be predicted by parental depression and anxiety. Note that this was tested separately for each variable to avoid problems with multicollinearity. The only significant effect that was found was for depression on invalidating responses (z=2.89). The model with all significant paths only is depicted in Figure 2. The fit indices of this model were: χ2(142) = 296.52, CFI = .901, RMSEA = .074.

Figure 2.Standardized paths in the final model predicting childhood somatic complaints. Arrows indicate the assumed causal direction of the path.
 Standardized paths in the final model predicting childhood somatic complaints. Arrows indicate the assumed causal direction of the path.

Discussion

In this study, effects of emotional and parental factors on childhood somatic complaints were examined. A particular strength of this study was that all variables were addressed simultaneously in a full model. Because of this, direct as well as indirect effects could be examined. The aim of the study to combine the two paths of previous research was successful. The results of the study show that childhood negative affect and parental somatic complaints are positively associated with childhood somatic complaints. In turn, childhood negative affect is related to children’s worrying and to parents responses to children’s emotions. The more anxious or depressed children felt, the more they worried. Invalidating parental responses to child emotions were positively related to depression whereas validating responses were negatively related to depression. It was also found that parents who experienced more negative affect, reported more somatic complaints and tended to report more invalidating responses towards their children’s emotions. As such, the results confirm the expectations as support the model presented in Figure 1.

Associations Between Negative Affect and Somatic Complaints

Whereas for children, feelings of depression and anxiety showed independent relationships with somatic complaints, in the parentsonly a direct path from anxiety towards somatic complaintswas found. This result must be cautiously interpreted, however, as previous research with a larger sample has found a relationship between somatic complaints and anxiety as well as depression 17. The relationships between anxiety, depression, and somatic complaints were stronger for the parents than for the children. Previous research in adults has shown that individuals with anxiety tend to develop depression and that there is high comorbidity between anxiety and depression 42. This interrelatedness may explain why it is difficult to find separate effects for anxiety and depression.

Interestingly, the relationship between worry and somatic complaints was indirect: in adults it seemed to be mediated by anxiety and in children by anxiety and depression. It has been shown that worry can cause cardiac changes independent of mood changes 43. In this way, worry can evoke similar physiological responses compared to cognitive solving activity. Similarly, it has been found that appraisal and repetitive negative thoughts are related to levels of cortisol (a stress-released hormone) and immune reactivity to stress 44. The current results demonstrate, however, that it is not just cognitive involvement that increases the risk of somatic complaints. Symptoms of anxiety or depression are better predictors, which are in turn positively related to worry.

Whether individuals develop symptoms of depression or anxiety depends on cognitive factors, but also on biological factors (e.g., genetic, hormonal) and emotional factors (e.g., emotional reactivity or the in this study found (in)valididating responses received) 45, 46, 47. Future studies could further investigate the precise role of depression and anxiety. For example, symptom perception might be influenced by depression44. Further, it has been suggested that not cortisol and (related) immune reactions to stress, but autonomic nervous system dysfunction (such as enteric nervous system dysfunction in the case of abdominal pain) explain the link between negative affect and somatic symptoms 48, 49.

Parental Factors

The current results further clearly demonstrate that in so far as parental reactions to child emotions are associated with somatic complaints, this relationship is indirect. As was expected, maladaptive responses to children’s emotions by parents are associated with more negative affect in children.

Parents also have a more direct influence through their own somatic complaints and this effect should be further investigated. Previous research supported that at least part of the relationship between parent and child somatic complaints is genetic 50. Recently, a new focus on genetic influences has emerged. The complexity of genetic influences is now acknowledged. It seems that that are multiple (genetic) factors that influence all or specific somatic symptoms and depression and anxiety 51. In addition, the need for an understanding of gene x environment effects is stressed 51, 52.

Study Limitations

There are some limitations of this study that should be taken into consideration. First of all, a community sample was used. It was,therefore, impossibleto investigate depression, anxiety, or somatic complaints within the clinical range. In addition, we had no access to parent’s or children’s medical records. This might have resulted in an underestimation of effects on somatic complaints. After all, some complaints that were reported might be mainly explained by medical causes. A final limitation that needs to bestressed is the low response rate in parental participation. Although no differences in the child reports were found, it might be that parents who experience parenting problems are more reluctant to answer questions about how they respond to child emotions than parents who experience no such problems. This may have let to weaker associations between parental reactions to child emotions and our other variables than would have been found in the total population.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the current study has integrated some of the previous findings on childhood somatic complaints by combining effects of negative affect and parental factors in one model. The results confirm that there is a direct association between parental and child somatic complaints. Besides this, both child and parental somatic complaints are related to negative affect. Because parents with negative affect also are more likely to show maladaptive responses to children’s emotions, they also indirectly further increase the likelihood of children’s somatic complaints through childhood negative affect.

References

  1. 1.Annequin D, Tourniaire B, Massiou H. (2000) Migraine and headache in childhood and adolescence. , Pediatric Clinics of North America 47(3), 617.
  1. 2.Brattberg G. (1994) The incidence of back pain and headache among Swedish school-children. , Quality of Life Research. Dec;3 27-31.
  1. 3.Perquin C W, Hazebroek-Kampschreur A, Hunfeld J A M, Bohne A M, Suijlekom-Smit L W A van. (2000) Pain in children and adolescents a common experience Pain.Jul;. 87(1), 51-8.
  1. 4.Staal N G, AAM Crijnen, Dopfner M, Verhulst F C. (1999) Physical complaints in children in Germany and in the Netherlands. , MonatsschriftKinderheilkunde.Feb; 147(2), 122-7.
  1. 5.Sundblad G M B, Saartok T, Engstrom L M T. (2007) Prevalence and co-occurrence of self-rated pain and perceived health in school-children, Age and gender differences EurJPain.Feb;. 11(2), 171-80.
  1. 6.Gilleland J, Suveg C, M L Jacob, Thomassin K. (2009) Understanding the medically unexplained, emotional and familial influences on children's somatic functioning. Child Care Health Dev. 35(3), 383-90.
  1. 7.F C Jellesma, Rieffe C, Meerum Terwogt M. (2008) My peers, my friend, and I, Peer interactions and somatic complaints in boys and girls. Soc Sc Med.Jun;. 66(11), 2195-205.
  1. 8.Masi G, Favilla L, Millepiedi S, Mucci M. (2000) Somatic symptoms in children and adolescents referred for emotional and behavioral disorders. , Psychiatry 63(2), 140-9.
  1. 9.J E Beck. (2008) A developmental perspective on functional somatic symptoms. , J Pediatr Psychol 33(5), 547-62.
  1. 10.J V Campo, Bridge J, Ehmann M, Altman S, Lucas. (2004) Recurrent abdominal pain, anxiety, and depression in primary care. , Pediatrics 113(4), 817-24.
  1. 11.L M Dufton, M J Dunn, B E Compas. (2009) Anxiety and Somatic Complaints in Children with Recurrent Abdominal Pain and Anxiety Disorders. , J Pediatr Psychol.Mar; 34(2), 176-86.
  1. 12.K S White, A D Farrell. (2006) Anxiety and psychosocial stress as predictors of headache and abdominal pain in urban early adolescents. , JPediatrPsychol.Jul; 31(6), 582-96.
  1. 13.F C Jellesma, Verkuil B, J F Brosschot. (2009) Postponing worrisome thoughts in children, The effects of a postponement intervention on perseverative thoughts, emotions and somatic complaints. Soc Sci Med. 69(2), 278-84.
  1. 14.J F Brosschot, Gerin W, J F Thayer. (2006) The perseverative cognition hypothesis, A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. , J Psychosom Res.Feb; 60(2), 113-24.
  1. 15.D A Axelson, Birmaher B. (2001) Relation between anxiety and depressive disorders in childhood and adolescence.Depress Anxiety.;. 14(2), 67-78.
  1. 16.C F Weems, W K Silverman, M La Greca A. (2000) What do youth referred for anxiety problems worry about? Worry and its relation to anxiety and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. , J Abnorm Child Psychol.Feb; 28(1), 63-72.
  1. 17.Haug T T, Mykletun A, Dahl A A. (2004) The association between anxiety, depression, and somatic symptoms in a large population, The HUNT-II study. Psychosom Med.Nov-Dec;. 66(6), 845-51.
  1. 18.J V Campo, Bridge J, Ehmann M, Altman S, Lucas A. (2004) Recurrent abdominal pain, anxiety, and depression in primary care. , Pediatrics.Apr; 113(4), 817-24.
  1. 19.N A Gillespie, Zhu G, A C Heath, I B Hickie, N G Martin. (2000) The genetic aetiology of somatic distress. , Psychol Med.Sep; 30(5), 1051-61.
  1. 20.Craig T K J, A D Cox, Klein K. (2002) Intergenerational transmission of somatization behaviour, a study of chronic somatizers and their children. , Psychol Med.Jul; 32(5), 805-16.
  1. 21.L S Walker, R L Claar, Garber J. (2002) Social consequences of children's pain, When do they encourage symptom maintenance?. , J Pediatr Psychol.Dec; 27(8), 689-98.
  1. 22.Whitehead W E, Crowell M D, Heller B R, J C Robinson, Schuster M M. (1994) Modeling and reinforcement of the sick role during childhood predicts adult illness behavior. , Psychosom Med.Nov-Dec; 56(6), 541-50.
  1. 23.E K Groholt, Stigum H, Nordhagen R, Kohler L. (2003) Recurrent pain in children, socio-economic factors and accumulation in families.EurJEpidemiol.;. 18(10), 965-75.
  1. 24.R N Jamison, L S Walker. (1992) Illness behavior in children of chronic pain patients. , International Journal of Psychiatry inMedicine.; 22(4), 329-42.
  1. 25.Hagekull B, Bohlin G. (2004) Predictors of middle childhood psychosomatic problems, An emotion regulation approach. , Infant Child Dev.Dec; 13(5), 389-405.
  1. 26.Eisenberg N, Fabes R A, Schaller M, Carlo G, Miller P A. (1991) The relations of parental characteristics and practices to children's vicarious emotional responding. , Child Dev.Dec; 62(6), 1393-408.
  1. 27.F C Jellesma. (2008) Health in young people, Social inhibition and negative affect and their relationship with self-reported somatic complaints. , Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics.Apr; 29(2), 94-100.
  1. 28.R A Fabes, S A Leonard, Kupanoff K, C L Martin. (2001) Parental coping with children’s negative emotions, Relations with children's emotional and social responding. Child Dev.May-Jun;. 72(3), 907-20.
  1. 29.Yap M B H, N B Allen, C D Ladouceur. (2008) Maternal socialization of positive affect, The impact of invalidation on adolescent emotion regulation and depressive symptomatology. , Child Dev 79(5), 1415-31.
  1. 30.R A Fabes, R E Poulin, Eisenberg N, A Madden Derdich D. (2002) The coping with children's negative emotions scale (CCNES), Psychometric properties and relations with children's emotional competence. , Marriage & FamilyReview.; 34(3), 285-310.
  1. 31.Kovacs M. (1992) Children's Depression Inventory Manual. , NorthTonawanda, NY, Multi-Health Systems,Inc
  1. 32.Timbremont B, Braet C. (2001) Psychometrische evaluatie van de Nederlandstalige Children's Depression Inventory. , Gedragstherapie.; 34(3), 229-242.
  1. 33.J S March, Parker J D A, Sullivan K, Stallings P, Conners C K. (1997) The multidimensional anxiety scale for children (MASC), Factor structure, reliability, and validity. , J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr.Apr; 36(4), 554-65.
  1. 34.EMWJ Utens, Ferdinand R F. (MASC-NL). Rotterdam, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Erasmus Medical Center Rotterdam/Sophia Children's Hospital (2000) Dutch translation of the March's Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children.
  1. 35.W Van Gastel, R F Ferdinand. (2008) Screening capacity of the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) for DSM‐IV anxiety disorders. Dep Anxiety.;. 25(12), 1046-1052.
  1. 36.F C Jellesma, Rieffe C, Terwogt M M. (2007) The somatic complaint list, Validation of a self-report questionnaire assessing somatic complaints in children. , J Psychosom Res. Oct; 63(4), 399-401.
  1. 37.R A Fabes, R E Poulin, Eisenberg N, Madden-Derdich D A. (2002) The coping with children's negative emotions scale (CCNES), Psychometric properties and relations with children's emotional competence. , Marriage & Family Review; 34(3), 285-310.
  1. 38.A T Beck, R A Steer, M G Garbin. (1988) Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory, 25 years of evaluation. , Clin PsycholRev.; 8(1), 77-100.
  1. 39.Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. (1970) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. , Palo Alto, CA,ConsultingPsychologistsPress
  1. 40.Meyer T, Miller M, Metzger R, Borkovec T. (1990) Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behav Res Ther.;28. 487-95.
  1. 41.R J Vandenberg, C E Lance. (2000) A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature, Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. , Organizational Research Methods.;3 4-69.
  1. 42.Yuan K-. (2005) Fit indices versus test statistics. , Multivariate Behav Res; 40(1), 115-48.
  1. 43.K R Merikangas, H P Zhang, Avenevoli S, Acharyya S, Neuenschwander M et al. (2003) Longitudinal trajectories of depression and anxiety in a prospective community study - The Zurich cohort study. , Arch Gen Psychiatry.Oct; 60(10), 993-1000.
  1. 44.Verkuil B, J F Brosschot, T D Borkovec, J F Thayer. (2009) Acute autonomic effects of experimental worry and cognitive problem solving, Why worry about worry?. , Int J Clin Health Psychol.Sep; 9(3), 439-53.
  1. 45.T F Denson, Spanovic M, Miller N. (2009) Cognitive Appraisals and Emotions Predict Cortisol and Immune Responses, A Meta-Analysis of Acute Laboratory Social Stressors and Emotion Inductions. Psychol Bull.Nov;. 135(6), 823-53.
  1. 46.B L Hankin, L Y Abramson, Miller N, G J Haeffel. (2004) Cognitive vulnerability-stress theories of depression, Examining affective specificity in the prediction of depression versus anxiety in three prospective studies. , Cogn Ther Res.Jun; 28(3), 309-45.
  1. 47.J S Hyde, A H Mezulis, L Y Abramson. (2008) The ABCs of depression, Integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. , Psychol Rev.Apr; 115(2), 291-313.
  1. 48.M B Howren, Suls J, Martin R. (2009) Depressive Symptomatology, Rather than Neuroticism, Predicts Inflated Physical Symptom Reports in Community-Residing Women. Psychosom Med.Nov-Dec;. 71(9), 951-7.
  1. 49.R B Lydiayd. (2001) Irritable bowel syndrome, anxiety, and depression, What are the links?. , J Clin Psychiatry.;62 38-47.
  1. 50.Huss D, Derefinko K, Milich R, Farzam F, Baumann R. (2009) Examining the Stress Response and Recovery Among Children With Migraine. , J Pediatr Psychol.Aug; 34(7), 707-15.
  1. 51.R L Levy, K R Jones, Whitehead W E, Feld S I, Talley N J et al. (2001) Irritable bowel syndrome in twins, Heredity and social learning both contribute to etiology. , Gastroenterology.Oct; 121(4), 799-804.
  1. 52.Kato K, P F Sullivan, Evengard B, N L Pedersen. (2009) A population-based twin study of functional somatic syndromes. , Psychol Med.Mar; 39(3), 497-505.
  1. 53.E de Geus. (2009) Genetic pschophysiological studies can inform theories in psychosomatic medicine. PsychophysSep;46, S16-

Cited by (2)

  1. 1.Meng Kun, Yuan Yizhe, Wang Yali, Liang Jianning, Wang Lijun, et al, 2020, Effects of parental empathy and emotion regulation on social competence and emotional/behavioral problems of school‐age children, Pediatric Investigation, 4(2), 91, 10.1002/ped4.12197
  1. 2.Glaus Jennifer, Moser Dominik A., Rusconi Serpa Sandra, Jouabli Sondes, Turri Fiorella, et al, 2022, Families With Violence Exposure and the Intergenerational Transmission of Somatization, Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13(), 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.820652